• 🇳🇿 🇲🇲 🇯🇵 🇨🇳 🇦🇺 🇦🇶 🇮🇳
    Australian & Asian
    Drug Discussion


    Welcome Guest!
    Posting Rules Bluelight Rules
  • AADD Moderators: andyturbo

NEWS: Report of Ecstasy Drug's Great Risks Is Retracted 06/09/03

Probably... if two bottles were delivered to the same lab for different experiments, or different experiment groups, then chances are that would be the 'drug bottle' which would be locked up and each injection sample would be taken from the same bottle, possibly by the same person (I think). Therefore the errors would be systematic (and hence, easier to trace in the event of anomalous results etc.) which is important in scientific research. If a researcher uses haphazard methods or varies their technique every time, then they'll never achieve any consistency (bad or good) in their results.

What am I getting at? If I personally was going to repeat an experiment to 'make sure' I would endeavour to follow exactly the same procedure, in order to make both sets of results comparable. If they were collected under different methods or circumstances, it's very hard to compare the results (this is an issue when aiming to reproduce the results of other people too, which is why science journal articles are written the 'standard' way they are).

BigTrancer :)
 
Ricuarte, G.A., Yuan, J., Hatzidimitriou, G., Cord, B.J. and McCann, U.D. "Retraction", Science, Letters to the Editor, p1479, (12 Sept 2003).

Retraction

We write to retract our report "Severe dopaminergic neurotoxicity in primates after a common recreational dose regimen of MDMA ("ecstasy")" (1), following our recent discovery that the drug used to treat all but one animal in that report came from a bottle that contained (+)-methamphetamine instead of the intended drug, (±)MDMA. Notably, (+)-methamphetamine would be expected to produce the same pattern of combined dopaminergic/serotonergic neurotoxicity (2) as that seen in the animals reported in our paper (1).

The originally published report (1) presented results from multiple studies performed in our laboratory over a span of approximately 2 years demonstrating that a novel systemic dose regimen of what we believed was MDMA produced severe dopamine neurotoxicity in two species of nonhuman primates, in addition to the previously reported serotonin neurotoxicity (3-6). Subsequent to the publication of those findings, we were unable to extend the dopamine neurotoxicity to orally administered doses. Multiple subsequent attempts to reproduce the original findings with systemically administered doses of MDMA identical to those used in the original study were also unsuccessful, under a variety of laboratory conditions.

We then noted that our studies aimed at extending and replicating the finding of MDMA-induced dopamine neurotoxicity were performed using a new batch of MDMA. This new batch of MDMA was determined to be authentic by several methods, including gas chromatography/ mass spectrometry (GC/MS).

Upon investigation of our laboratory records, we determined that the studies detailed in our paper (1) utilized a batch of MDMA that had been requested on the same date as a batch of (+)-methamphetamine and that both drug requests were for the same amount (10 g) and were processed by the supplier on the same day. Both drugs were delivered to our laboratory on the same day, in the same package. At delivery, the two bottles had different affixed labels, the same delivery reference number, but different batch numbers, as specified in their respective chemical data sheets. Following receipt, both drugs were stored in our laboratory in their original containers, in a locked safe.

When we began to suspect that the two bottles of drug might have borne incorrect labels [i.e., that the putative (±)MDMA was actually (+)-methamphetamine, and vice versa], we requested that a sample of the drug in the bottle bearing the original and intact label of "(+)-methamphetamine HCl" be analyzed by various analytical techniques, including GC/MS. Three independent laboratories found the sample to consist of MDMA, with no evidence of even trace amounts of methamphetamine.

Although the drug sample used in our original studies (1) was depleted and the empty bottle labeled MDMA had been discarded, we did have frozen brains from two animals that died shortly after drug treatment during the course of the original experiments (1). When these brains were analyzed by GC/MS by three independent laboratories, they were found to contain methamphetamine and its metabolite amphetamine, neither of which is a metabolite of MDMA (7). Not even trace amounts of MDMA or its metabolite MDA were found in these brains. Detailed review of our laboratory records revealed that all but one animal in our study (1) had been treated with the drug in the bottle labeled "(±)-methylenedioxymethamphetamine HCl" (MDMA) processed on the same date as the bottle labeled "(+)d-methamphetamine HCl."

This apparent labeling error does not call into question the results of multiple previous studies demonstrating the serotonin neurotoxic potential of MDMA in various animal species, including several nonhuman primate species (3-6, 8). Regarding the dopamine neurotoxic potential of MDMA in nonhuman primates, it remains possible that dose regimens in the range of those used by some humans, but different from those thus far tested, produce dopamine neurotoxicity in primates, as they do in rodents (9, 10). Moreover, lasting effects of MDMA on dopaminergic function in humans have recently been reported (11), and some humans with a history of MDMA abuse have developed Parkinsonism (12-14). However, until the dopamine neurotoxic potential of MDMA in primates can be examined more fully, this possibility remains uncertain.

George A. Ricaurte,
Department of Neurology,
Johns Hopkins Bayview Medical Center,
Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine,
Baltimore, MD 21224,
USA.

Jie Yuan,
Department of Neurology,
Johns Hopkins Bayview Medical Center,
Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine,
Baltimore, MD 21224,
USA.

George Hatzidimitriou,
Department of Neurology,
Johns Hopkins Bayview Medical Center,
Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine,
Baltimore, MD 21224,
USA.

Branden J. Cord,
Department of Neurosciences,
Johns Hopkins Bayview Medical Center,
Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine,
Baltimore, MD 21224,
USA.

Una D. McCann
Department of Psychiatry,
Johns Hopkins Bayview Medical Center,
Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine,
Baltimore, MD 21224,
USA.

References and Notes

1. G. A. Ricaurte, J. Yuan, G. Hatzidimitriou, B. J. Cord, U. D. McCann, Science 297, 2260 (2002).
2. V. Villemagne et al., J. Neurosci. 18, 419 (1998).
3. G. A. Ricaurte, L. E. Delanney, I. Irwin, J. W. Langston, Brain Res. 446, 165 (1988).
4. T. R. Insel, G. Battaglia, J. N. Johannessen, S. Marra, E. B. DeSouza, J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 249, 713 (1989).
5. M. S. Kleven, W. L. Woolverton, L. S. Seiden, Brain Res. 488, 121 (1989).
6. D. L. Frederick et al., Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 17, 531 (1995).
7. A. Cho, Y. Kumagai, in Amphetamine and its Analogs: Neuropsychopharmacology, Toxicology and Abuse, A. Cho, D. Segal, Eds. (Academic Press, New York, 1994), pp. 43-77.
8. W. Slikker Jr. et al., Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol. 94, 448 (1988).
9. D. L. Commins et al., J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 241, 338 (1987).
10. E. O'Shea, B. Esteban, J. Camarero, A. R. Green, M. I. Colado, Neuropharmacology 40, 65 (2001).
11. G. Gerra et al., Behav. Brain Res. 134, 403 (2002).
12. S. Mintzer, S. Hickenbottom, S. Gilman, N. Engl. J. Med. 340, 1443 (1999).
13. S. M. Kuniyoshi, J. Jankovic, N. Engl. J. Med. 349, 96 (2003).
14. G. Ricaurte, unpublished data.
15. We gratefully acknowledge helpful discussions with J. Katz, W. L. Hearn, and N. Ator. We are also grateful for the expert chemical analytical assistance of R. Fernandez, T. L. DalCason, M. Courtney, M. Daggett, I. Carroll and associates, and R. Foltz.


That's quite well explained, I think. Indeed, it appears this retraction follows exhaustive troubleshooting and investigative inquiries into the anomaly in the research programme.

BigTrancer :)
 
I would like to hear more about the people who have developed symtoms of Parkinsonism, or even read the reports based on these studies if thats possible.

Does anyone have a link for that info?

Thanks,

PinkangA
 
The error is well explained but he could have left it at that - instead he puts his two bob in at the end about various aspects of MDMA and its toxic potential when he should have said nothing.

ITs like saying well we convicted the wrong guy for murder as the DNA samples were mixed up.
But dont worry we know he did this rape last year that we got him for and we are already assuming he committed this recent armed holdup too.
So because of the previous offence and the one we know he did, our mistake is not really of much significance.


I'm still trying to get over the doses of methamphetamine given - i bet the monkey given the MDMA not only survived but is still running around quite happy; unless the poor sod has since been subjected to more tests.
 
so they were supposed to receive 3-4 130mg hits of MDMA with a 3 hour gap inbetween each hit? jesus, while that is fucking stupid amounts of meth they received, that is still a ridiculous amount of MDMA over a short amount of time.
i know people out there do eat like 10 pills a night.......but that's due to tollerance. i think the monkey that apparently got the MDMA would have been bloody uncomfortable.
why use such extreme dosages? also, a primate would have a different body mass but most importantly, their basal metabolic rate would/maybe different to humans.
did they even take these basic concepts into consideration?
also, wouldn't their paper had to of been peer reviewed? what happened there?
 
AS i understand it it was 6mg injected per 2 pounds body weight, done 3 times three hours apart.
I and others have assumed it must be the total divided 3 times. A 150pound person gets 450mg over 9 hours; so thats 150mg each time.

You are right - it is ridiculous; most people i know would not take 3 of the best pills in history in that amount of time. Thats like a decent pill (75mg) every 90mins over 9 hours.
What then blows that effort of "realism" out of the water is the fact it was injected - u can double the effective dose right there; i dont know what sort of drug users these researchers are acquainted with - drug monsters more like it.
Pass the 150mg pills my way by all means; but dont expect me to have more than 1.5 and expect them to be swallowed.

Considering primates have similar digestive systems, why these drugs cannot be administered orally is beyond me. Call me a cynic but it is probably because they simply wouldnt do the required requisite of damage.
 
Bit of a Saturday piece for The Age but news of the retraction appeared on their online version today.

Ecstasy 'time bomb' warning just a fizzer
By Jo Revill
September 13, 2003

It was billed as the one of the most dramatic warnings the world has ever received over the dangers of ecstasy. A study from one of America's leading universities concluded that taking the drug for just one evening could leave users with irreversible brain damage and trigger the onset of Parkinson's disease.

The study, published in the eminent journal Science last September, had an immediate impact. Doctors and anti-drug crusaders spoke of a "neurological time bomb" facing the young. Others suggested that taking one tablet was the equivalent of playing Russian roulette with the brain, and demanded tighter anti-rave laws. The article's findings made newspapers around the world, including The Age.

But this week scientists had to admit that the stark results they reported were not a breakthrough but a humiliating blunder.

The study was based on the fact that laboratory monkeys and baboons had a severe reaction to the drug when it was injected in small doses. But it has now emerged that the vials of liquid did not contain ecstasy. Instead, the animals received a dose of methamphetamine, or speed - a drug widely known to affect the body's dopamine system. The tubes had somehow been mislabelled by the supplier.

In this week's Science, the scientists have published a retraction of their original study, reigniting the row over the role of those who investigate ecstasy, as well as the real risks or benefits of the drug.

In academic circles, the mistake is a severe embarrassment to Johns Hopkins University, in Baltimore, Maryland, which attracts millions of dollars of research funding. Questions are already being asked about whether the lead researcher, George Ricaurte, was inherently biased against the drug.

The mistake came to light when follow-up tests gave conflicting results. The original study reported that two out of 10 animals died quickly after their second or third dose. Six weeks later, the dopamine levels in the surviving animals were down 65 per cent, leading Dr Ricaurte and his colleagues to conclude that it could provoke the onset of Parkinson's, which is linked to dopamine-producing cell loss.

When the study was published, experts saw it as evidence of drug damage. Dr Alan Leshner, chief executive of the American Academy for the Advancement of Science, which publishes the journal, described taking ecstasy as playing Russian roulette with brain function.

He said: "This study showed that even very occasional use can have long-lasting effects on many different brain systems."

Yesterday, Dr Ricaurte was trying to put a brave face on the calamity. He has already been accused of rushing his study into print because Congress was looking at a bill known as the Anti-Rave Act, which would punish club owners who knew that drugs such as ecstasy were being used on their premises.

Dr Ricaurte has denied political bias. He said yesterday that his laboratory made "a simple human error".

Asked why the vials of liquid were not checked before being used on the animals, he replied: "We're not chemists. We get hundreds of chemicals here - it's not customary to check them."

Controversy has raged about the dangers of ecstasy. Some studies suggest that ecstasy has no long-term effect on the levels of the hormone serotonin in the brain, while others suggest it leaves users depressed and unable to concentrate.

- Observer

Link
 
Psychadelic_Paisly said:
so they were supposed to receive 3-4 130mg hits of MDMA with a 3 hour gap inbetween each hit? jesus, while that is fucking stupid amounts of meth they received, that is still a ridiculous amount of MDMA over a short amount of time.
i know people out there do eat like 10 pills a night.......but that's due to tollerance. i think the monkey that apparently got the MDMA would have been bloody uncomfortable.
why use such extreme dosages? also, a primate would have a different body mass but most importantly, their basal metabolic rate would/maybe different to humans.
did they even take these basic concepts into consideration?
also, wouldn't their paper had to of been peer reviewed? what happened there?

These questions can, in part, be answered by going through the original article: http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/short/297/5590/2260

From Ricuarte et al. (2003), Science Volume 297, Number 5590, Issue of 27 Sep 2002, pp. 2260-2263.

We report severe, functionally significant dopaminergic neurotoxicity, along with more modest serotonergic neurotoxicity, in primates treated with doses of MDMA modeled after those commonly used by recreational MDMA users (This statement is what is being retracted, BT). Earlier studies in nonhuman primates have generally involved administration of higher MDMA doses (5 or 10 mg/kg) twice daily (morning and evening) for 4 consecutive days. These dosage regimens typically engendered more severe but highly selective toxicity toward brain serotonin neurons, with no long-term effects on brain dopamine neurons (16-18) (Hindsight: because MDMA was used, BT). Because the drug regimens used in previous studies did not model those used by most MDMA users, the possibility remained that occasional MDMA users might not be at risk for neurotoxic injury. The present results, however, indicate that even individuals who use MDMA on one occasion may be at risk for substantial brain injury if they use two or three sequential doses, hours apart, as is often the case in recreational settings.

In the present studies, MDMA was given by a systemic route (subcutaneously in squirrel monkeys and intramuscularly in baboons), whereas humans generally take MDMA orally. It is possible that humans are at a decreased risk for neurotoxic injury because of differences in the route of administration. However, in the case of MDMA, oral administration offers little or no significant neuroprotection (19-22). Even if some degree of protection were afforded by oral administration, the profound loss of dopaminergic neuronal markers seen in both primate species suggests that significant neurotoxicity would still occur. Moreover, individual doses of MDMA used in this study are lower than those typically used by humans (1.6 to 2.4 mg/kg), once adjusted with interspecies dose scaling methods (23). Hence, any protection that might be associated with oral administration would likely be offset by increasing the dose of MDMA used in this study to the human equivalent. It is not uncommon for recreational MDMA users to use repeated doses of the drug on more than one occasion or more than two or three repeated doses per session.

So the short answer is that these doses look HUGE to us, but the metabolic rates and interspecies differences are taken into account by a dose scaling factor. This is the same method that is used to calculate LD50 levels in people, by injecting increasing doses into laboratory mouse species, and other animal testing, and then scaling the dose from that species to an approximate human LD50. Obviously, we cannot ethically do studies on people, so this is the way that human doses are calculated in animal studies customarily.

This study would definitely have been peer-reviewed with careful scrutiny, since Science is one of the highest-impact, premier scholarly journals; the results are NOT flawed in any way. However, unfortunately the systematic errors in their study caused by the administration of the wrong chemical led them to anomalous conclusions. So the results are indeed accurate in that injection of that chemical induced dopaminergic neurotoxicity, but the assumption and extension of these results to MDMA users in general led to inaccurate interpretation of that result, and widespread media interest. There will be red-faces for sure, but most of it is due to media misinterpretation of extrapolated exaggeration, IMO. There's part of me that hopes Ricuarte and collaborators will now redouble their efforts to prove conclusively and definitively their hypotheses in all further publications: there could be some awesome science produced from that laboratory in the near future in efforts to save "public reputations". I do not imagine that any scientist ever wants to publish more than 1 retraction in a career.

BigTrancer :)
 
I have an audio recording of Ricaurte's presentation of this work at the Ecstasy Conference in 2001. To be fair, it's actually pretty good although he's not the best speaker and was shot down by critics at question time.

The file's too big to host and D/L in it's current format. If you want it, or the whole show PM me and I'll consider making some copies if the number is small. Otherwise I might host via a free streaming account.
 
I still feel the dosing is too high.
1.5mg for a 70kg person is 105mg - how many people are injecting that 3 times over a night.
If they insist on injecting that dose should be halved.

Even if neurotoxicity is the same either route (and that flies in the face of everything i have ever learnt) people would be having smaller amounts; so the monkeys should get smaller amounts.

And the results are no way realistic for meth; lets say with scaling they were only doing a 1mg/kg human dose; who is bunting 1 point of street quality "ice" (80% meth) on each occasion every few hours - no one i know. And if they intend on doing studies to represent what the community is doing to themselves then they should be doing studies that reflect the average user in the community - not the most extreme (insane).

No one agrees more than me about MDMAs potential neurotoxicity - but Ricaurte's independence from the tyrannical US drug tzars is so questionable and this study is just another question mark - or at least in my mind its like someone accidentally lighting a beacon to this fact in an amusing sort of way
 
^^^^ totally agree.
it's like doing an experiment and coming to the conclusion that, while everyone knows eating ice cream makes you fat, if we give these monkey's TRUCK LOADS of ice cream (but tell the public it's a normal amount) making them violently ill, that ice cream is obviously evil and must be taken off our local mr. whippy's!!!!
 
i refer now to the theory put to us by the great Darwin, Survival of the fittest.

In Africa, tigers attack only the weak, old and dilapidated members of bison herds, because therre lies strength in numbers, and dawdling, struggling prey is easy to pick off.
Thus, the whole herd is now faster and stronger, having relinquished the slower members of their herd.

Now apply this to our brains. Take drugs and alcohol and we kill the slower, less used brain cells, thus making our brain as a unit, faster, stronger and more efficient.

KEEP POPPING!
 
Unfortunately by that analogy, since when you're drunk your whole brain slows down at once, the alcohol monsters that eat those brain cells can be indiscriminately munching on the whole slow lot...

BigTrancer ;)
 
Apoptosis is the process whereby cells are preprogrammed to die. Some cells normally exist for longer than others while some are destroyed before they have aged. For many types of neurons, the exact mechanisms involved in neural apoptosis have not been deduced.

However, it is known than excitatory neurotransmitters such as glutamate, signal the destruction of some neurons by acting as an "integrity assessor". Very basically, in Alzheimers disease, glutamate is released in larger amounts in parts of the Cerebellum. This has the affect of "labeling" nicotinic receptors for premature destruction. Some Alzheimers drugs protect these neurons by altering the firing (modulation) of the receptor, causing the neuron to be ignored by glutamate and so not be destroyed. nicotine is thought to act in the same way.

Many psychotropic drugs cause increases in excitatory neurotransmitters, released as a result of increases in enzymes, or simply to initiate destruction of cells "weakened" by depletion of certain proteins, damage from free radicals etc.

It therefore makes sense that a combination of drugs could cause a greater range of cells to be weakened and therefore be more susceptible to being labeled for destruction. It's important to note that many excitatory chemicals are not that selective, and may cause the destruction of different types of cells.
 
Poor Ricuart, his scientific credibility, already in the dumps, has now been completely destroyed. I doubt he will ever been seen in a serious light ever again.

Just goes to show what happens when you live science for politics. His attitude and scientific direction angers me, so I don't feel remorse when I say that he "sucked research dick" for politics.
:p

He gets what he deserves...
 
dexter_stayne - You would be surprised as to how complex adaptation is. What you state is only a small portion of the reality of fitness and adaptation.

Fit individuals sometimes take chances to protect the weak by tricking the predator, feining weakness, then running off. Hehehe. But I get your point. ;)
 
leprechaun - was only meant as a rhetoric, not to be taken seriously.

your kidding if you think that extended usage of ecstasy won't do ANY harm. Even a slight personality change can be an indication of damage.
 
An Interesting Summary of Ricaurte's work

From: New York Times: 2nd Dec, 2003

Research on Ecstasy Is Clouded by Errors

By DONALD G. McNEIL Jr.

Published: December 2, 2003


In September, the journal Science issued a startling retraction.

A primate study it published in 2002, with heavy publicity, warned that the amount of the drug Ecstasy that a typical user consumes in a single night might cause permanent brain damage.

It turned out that the $1.3 million study, led by Dr. George A. Ricaurte of Johns Hopkins University, had not used Ecstasy at all. His 10 squirrel monkeys and baboons had instead been injected with overdoses of methamphetamine, and two of them had died. The labels on two vials he bought in 2000, he said, were somehow switched.

The problem corrupted four other studies in his lab, forcing him to withdraw four other papers.

It was not the first time Dr. Ricaurte's lab was accused of using flawed studies to suggest that recreational drugs are highly dangerous. In previous years he was accused of publicizing doubtful results without checking them, and was criticized for research that contributed to a government campaign suggesting that Ecstasy made "holes in the brain."

Dr. Ricaurte, a 50-year-old neurologist at Hopkins since 1988, is probably the best-known Ecstasy expert in the war on drugs. He has received $10 million from the National Institute on Drug Abuse, more than any other investigator of the amphetamine analogs known as designer drugs, club drugs or diet drugs, including MDMA, better known as Ecstasy, and its close relative MDA.

He vigorously defends his work, saying much of it has been confirmed by other researchers, and arguing that he is often unfairly attacked by scientists who minimize the dangers of designer drugs because they want to use them in research.

Johns Hopkins stands behind him. "The institution has every confidence in his ability," said Gary Stevenson, a spokesman. Of the primate study, he said Dr. Ricaurte "made an honest mistake, then discovered it and revealed it."

But other scientists, and two human research subjects of Dr. Ricaurte's who came forward after the retraction, say they see a pattern of shaky research supporting alarmist press releases.

It is hard to find impartial observers in the highly politicized debate over illegal drugs. But even three scientists whom Dr. Ricaurte cited in his own defense said that while his high media profile had made him a "whipping boy" for those favoring Ecstasy research, some of his best-known work has nonetheless been "sloppy" or "not as methodologically rigorous as you might want."

Longtime critics are harsher.

"It's hard to trust George," said Dr. Julie Holland, a professor of psychiatry at New York University who has edited a book on Ecstasy and wants to test it in psychotherapy. She accused him of "playing games with his data" to win more federal grants by making the drugs look bad.

Dr. Richard J. Wurtman, a prominent clinician at Harvard and M.I.T. who has clashed with Dr. Ricaurte, accused him of "running a cottage industry showing that everything under the sun is neurotoxic."

For 20 years, Dr. Ricaurte has produced studies saying the amphetamine analogs may cause the tremors of Parkinsonism, depression and memory and sleep problems. But the consensus among many amphetamine researchers, Dr. Ricaurte included, is that there is no proof thus far that Ecstasy causes permanent human brain damage. In animal studies, very high doses have destroyed serotonin-pathway nerves, which convey pleasure and affect memory and appetite.

Just last month Dr. Stephen J. Kish of the Center for Addiction and Mental Health in Toronto published a review of all Ecstasy research, including Dr. Ricaurte's, and concluded that there was no evidence that Ecstasy caused the tremors of Parkinsonism or any other brain damage "with the possible (but as yet unproven) exception of mild memory loss."

Some heavy users have memory problems, but no studies prove the loss is permanent, or that it is caused by Ecstasy rather than other drugs in the mix that virtually all heavy users take.

Ecstasy — invented in Germany in 1912 by Merck Pharmaceuticals in its search for an anti-bleeding drug — has been outlawed in the United States since 1985, a decision that Dr. Ricaurte has taken partial credit for. Since about 1970, when it was called Adam, some psychiatrists had tried giving low doses to trauma victims; in 1985, they stopped, fearing arrest.


Dr. Holland says it relieves anxiety-provoking memories like a sedative, but as an amphetamine, it does not induce sleep. Patients "want to talk things through."

As a potent painkiller, she said, it also may help the terminally ill.

The Food and Drug Administration recently approved a study in traumatized crime victims who have failed to respond to antidepressants. A study of rape victims is under way in Spain, and another one in the United States is proposed for depressed patients with terminal cancer.

When Dr. Ricaurte's 2002 primate study was published, his critics said he could not possibly have given "typical recreational doses" if 2 of 10 animals died and two others collapsed of heatstroke.

According to an annual federal survey, almost 10 million Americans have tried Ecstasy. Few have died.

"Those dead animals should have sent up a red flag," said Dr. Charles R. Schuster, a former director of the national drug institute whom Dr. Ricaurte has called a mentor. "The better part of valor would have been to not publish until it was repeated."

Dr. Ricaurte said such arguments "do not hold water," since animal deaths are common in amphetamine research, and two is too few to compare to human death rates. Dr. Nora Volkow, the new director of the national drug institute, declined to pass judgment on his whole body of work, but called his latest error "crying wolf and losing your credibility." Because of it, she said, she spent a weekend checking the agency's Web page on the dangers of Ecstasy "to make sure it was not overstated."

The agency had already removed all current references to another well-known study from the site, one from 1998 by Dr. Ricaurte and his wife, Dr. Una McCann. Dr. Volkow described it as using "methodologies that were not optimal."

Pictures from the study — PET scans of the brains of Ecstasy users — were used on a famous postcard from the drug agency, "Plain Brain/Brain After Ecstasy." The postcards were distributed to thousands of teenagers and implied that Ecstasy users had shrunken brains with holes in them.

The study had nothing to do with holes, but with serotonin levels, which Dr. Ricaurte found drastically depleted in 14 subjects who had taken Ecstasy 70 to 400 times.

Dr. Marc Laruelle, a Columbia University PET scan specialist, called the work so technically flawed that it was "something to put under the rug." He cited a recent German study showing that serotonin decreased only modestly and returned to normal within six weeks. The Hopkins team, he said, presented its data in logarithmically compressed graphs that seemed calculated to mask the fact that it had found impossible results: its 15 "control" subjects had serotonin levels 50 times normal.

Dr. Ricaurte defended the study, saying his recalculation technique was common when results from two groups varied widely, although he said he no longer used it.

Of the photos, Dr. Ricaurte said he had no control over what the national drug institute did with his work, but he had asked an agency official to fix their "poor quality."

In the 1990's, Dr. Ricaurte was involved in a dispute over the danger of dexfenfluramine, another amphetamine analog sold in Europe as a prescription diet drug.

In 1994, a company founded by Dr. Wurtman, director of clinical research at the Harvard-M.I.T. health science division, sought F.D.A. permission to market it in the United States.

Dr. Ricaurte released a study saying it caused brain damage; that was immediately disputed by an Environmental Protection Agency study that found it did no permanent harm.

In September 1995, Dr. Mark E. Molliver, a Hopkins colleague who frequently published with Dr. Ricaurte, presented slides to an advisory committee of the Food and Drug Administration showing Alzheimer's-like brain tangles.

Dr. Wurtman, who contacted The New York Times after the Science article retraction, said that Dr. Molliver, with Dr. Ricaurte in the audience, misled the committee by implying the damage was done by dexfenfluramine.

In an interview, Dr. Molliver called that "a blatant lie," and asserted that he had clearly said he was showing damage done by similar drugs. Dr. Ricaurte agreed.


But transcripts of a follow-up hearing in November 1995 provided by Dr. Wurtman show that several panelists and the F.D.A.'s expert were confused and believed that Dr. Molliver had been showing dexfenfluramine damage. Ultimately the drug was not approved.

For a week in 1996, Greg M. was one of Dr. Ricaurte's lab subjects.

At the time, he said, he was using large amounts of Ecstasy, marijuana, LSD, cocaine, amphetamines and heroin.

After seeing the retraction of the primate study, he contacted The Times, and persuaded a friend who had accompanied him to call, too.

The two revealed their names and occupations but declined to be fully identified for fear their former drug use would hurt their careers. Greg is a graduate student in chemistry at a leading university. His friend, who said he used to follow Grateful Dead tours selling up to 10,000 doses of LSD a month, now works at a West Coast law firm and is in line for a federal job.

Curious to see if they had damaged their brains, and enticed by a promise of $100 a day and a free East Coast trip, they enlisted.

Although the two used many drugs, the research assistant who interviewed them by phone told them what not to admit to her if they wanted to be in the study, Greg said. They were instructed to avoid all drugs for three weeks to avoid tainting the study; Greg says he had used heroin five days earlier.

They and other Ecstasy users flown in from the West Coast took memory tests while still jet-lagged, they said.

Then after lumbar punctures to check serotonin levels, neither was given the usual night's rest to prevent fierce headaches. They had to carry their backpacks across campus and be wired up for a sleep study, which Greg argued could not reflect normal sleep patterns because they were in pain.

Both had subsequent tests after shots of morphine and a drug, mCPP, that causes the same eyeball twitching and teeth-grinding as Ecstasy, but none of the euphoria. Then they had PET scans.

Dr. Ricaurte said his research protocols are approved by university committees. He acknowledged testing sedated or jet-lagged subjects, but argued that he had always noted that limitation in his published papers, and switched to testing in early mornings when jet lag was minimal. Test subjects who get lumbar punctures are warned about headaches, and given rest and painkillers, he said.

To weed out subjects who confound results by using other drugs, Dr. Ricaurte said, his staff quizzed volunteers and did blood and urine tests. His papers acknowledge that hair tests, which can show many drugs taken even months back, would have been more accurate. (Dr. Laruelle, who does PET scans of Ecstasy users, rejects subjects with hair less than an inch long.)

Told that Greg had used heroin without getting caught, Dr. Ricaurte said that was "unfortunate." But like all drug researchers, he said it was impossible to find heavy Ecstasy users who used no other drugs.

His papers, he said, always warn that poor performance by heavy Ecstasy users may have been caused by other drugs.

His critics say that such fine-print disclaimers are not enough, that all mental tests on multiple-drug users are pointless and cannot be used as evidence that one particular drug damages the brain.

Greg's friend reiterated that he had been badly treated and said he felt the research was skewed to prove he was brain-damaged.

"Most of the people I used to do drugs with are pretty screwed up," he admitted. "But if Ricaurte's studies are true, Greg and I should both be dead. We ate grams a night of pharma-grade stuff."

Nonetheless, he said: "We're fairly intelligent, rational guys. We had a stretch of three or four years where we really blew ourselves out. But we're still smart and ambitious. Some of their assertions about long-term brain damage are way off."
 
Allright, there are several reasons why massive doses would be used, and why they would be injected rather than given orally.

Firstly - the drug is injected for one reason - to control exactly how much is absorbed. When you take it orally, parameters such as when you ate, acidity, etc. and also things like first-pass metabolism at the liver, affect the absorption, and so the doses aren't standard. Injection lets you give the monkeys exactly how much you want them to have. Plus, I'm guessing it's easier to inject a monkey than make it take a pill (ever wormed a cat? ;))

As to the large doses... firstly, different species have different metabolisms, and so larger doses may be needed to account for this (of course, this is more obvious in rodents and small animals, but differences will also be important even between primates).

In addition, it's far easier to see the results of massive doses than it is to see the effects of small doses. MDMA may or may not be neurotoxic, but it's more likely that significant, easily observable damage will occur if you give them a huge dose. This way, you can prove if the drug is toxic (although lack of damage does not prove non-toxicity, as it may take time for the damage to manifest...) and then you can do low-dose studies to see how regular doses affect the brain, using the results from the high-dose experiments as an indication of what sort of damage to look for.
 
I simply don't understand why they scale up the dosages given the body mass and metabolic rate. Pharmacologically it makes no sense. Especially with injection.

I know when I have had even a little too much of something. You feel it, you feel like something toxic is going through your body and your brain, you know something is wrong. A normal dose of MDMA has little of such impact. I have felt much worse the day after binging on Alcohol then on a 120mg + 50mg dose of MDMA. If so many people take MDMA, shouldn't there be an epidemic of depression? Most people I have talked to have little problems after they have decreased their intake of MDMA. :P Even with 200mg of MDMA I can tell something is wrong, it definetly feels like "too much", with sign of toxicity being evident.

Well, I guess its up to each of us to make our own decisions. I trust my nervous system far more then any scientific study, especially when it seems unreasonable and unrealistic. :) So for me MDMA is a wonderful substance that when used in moderation can act as the perfect catalyst for bringing people together and making a night out that something special.

:)
 
Top